Skip to main content

Immediate gain is long-term loss: Are there foresighted decision makers in the Iowa Gambling Task?

Abstract

Background

The Somatic Marker Hypothesis suggests that normal subjects are "foreseeable" and ventromedial prefrontal patients are "myopic" in making decisions, as the behavior shown in the Iowa Gambling Task. The present study questions previous findings because of the existing confounding between long-term outcome (expected value, EV) and gain-loss frequency variables in the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT). A newly and symmetrically designed gamble, namely the Soochow Gambling Task (SGT), with a high-contrast EV between bad (A, B) and good (C, D) decks, is conducted to clarify the issue about IGT confounding. Based on the prediction of EV (a basic assumption of IGT), participants should prefer to choose good decks C and D rather than bad decks A and B in SGT. In contrast, according to the prediction of gain-loss frequency, subjects should prefer the decks A and B because they possessed relatively the high-frequency gain.

Methods

The present experiment was performed by 48 participants (24 males and 24 females). Most subjects are college students recruited from different schools. Each subject played the computer version SGT first and completed a questionnaire for identifying their final preference. The IGT experimental procedure was mostly followed to assure a similar condition of decision uncertainty.

Results

The SGT experiment demonstrated that the prediction of gain-loss frequency is confirmed. Most subjects preferred to choose the bad decks A and B than good decks C and D. The learning curve and questionnaire data indicate that subjects can not "hunch" the EV throughout the game. Further analysis of the effect of previous choice demonstrated that immediate gain increases the probability to stay at the same deck.

Conclusion

SGT provides a balanced structure to clarify the confounding inside IGT and demonstrates that gain-loss frequency rather than EV guides decision makers in these high-ambiguity gambles. Additionally, the choice behavior is mostly following the "gain-stay, lose-randomize" strategy to cope with the uncertain situation. As demonstrated in SGT, immediate gain can bring about a long-term loss under uncertainty. This empirical result may explain some shortsighted behaviors in real life.

Background

Studies in behavioral decision-making and affective neuroscience have found that typical decision makers are frequently "myopic" [1–5] to long-term outcome (expected value, EV) [6]. Conversely, Damasio [7] and Bechara et al. [8, 9] proposed the Somatic Marker Hypothesis and conducted the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) to test whether ventromedial prefrontal patients are shortsighted in terms of the future and long-term outcome, and whether typical decision makers can predict or foresee the future. For comparison, the notion of shortsighted vs. foresighted is adopted in this study. A marked difference exists between shortsightedness and foresight. The IGT was the central test for a verification of the Somatic Marker Hypothesis. Some studies have attempted to replicate or modify the protocol used in the IGT [10–12]; whereas others have proposed that the hypothesis was theoretically inadequate [1, 5, 13–16]. Lin et al. [17] pointed out that there are increasing number of studies [18–28] to demonstrate a contradictory phenomenon, namely, the "prominent deck B phenomenon" [29]. The phenomenon showed that normal decision makers can not prevent their preference to "bad" (EV) deck B in the standard version of IGT (or, due to the effect of "gain-loss frequency"). Recently, Bechara (Sevy et al.) [30, 31] also revealed a "prominent deck B phenomenon". In their study, normal subjects preferred the bad deck B rather than the other three decks. Furthermore, the chosen number of deck B (31) in Sevy et al. study [30] is almost a double of Bechara et al. data in 1994 (about 17) [8]. On the other hand, Lin et al. [29] analyzed the existing experimental results adopting simple version of IGT and suggest that the "prominent deck B phenomenon" may be due to a confounding from gain-loss frequency. Additionally, Chiu and Lin [32] utilize a modified version of IGT to demonstrate that subjects' preference to deck C is also due to gain-loss frequency, not EV. A fundamental structural flaw exists in a failure of orthogonal separation between EV and gain-loss frequency (or, frequency of punishments and rewards). This limitation has not been properly evaluated in IGT literature. The study tries to explore the implications of this flaw.

Damasio's [7] Somatic Marker Hypothesis proposes that normal decision-making is often assisted by somatic markers. Ventromedial prefrontal patients are influenced largely by immediate reinforcement and are insensitive to, or cannot see, future consequences due to a lack of past affective experiences. Normal subjects with intact somatic markers benefit from repeated exposure to punishments and rewards when performing tasks and are cognizant of future outcome. Somatic states serve as a neural expression biasing the brain process when evaluating the badness or goodness of each decision. According to Damasio [7]:

"... the brains of the normal subjects were gradually learning to predict a bad outcome, and were signaling the relative badness of the particular deck before the actual card-turning." (Damasio, 1994, p 220).

In short, the Somatic Marker Hypothesis suggests that somatic markers, which are processed implicitly, can facilitate decision makers in making advantageous decisions [9] and guiding explicit decision making [33, 34].

The most compelling empirical evidence for supporting the Somatic Marker Hypothesis is found in the IGT [8]. Over the past decade, the IGT has been widely employed as a neuropsychological research instrument for investigating affective and executive function. At least 100 scientific studies have utilized the IGT to investigate a diverse set of neurological and psychiatric populations [35].

In the IGT, card decks A and B were designated "bad" decks with low EV ($ -250), and C and D were "good" decks with high EV ($ +250) in average 10 trials. Subjects and ventromedial prefrontal patients selected "bad" decks during the first 30 card-turning trials (out of 100) [7, 36]; however, normal subjects gradually shifted to "good" decks and avoided the "bad" decks [8, 9, 36–39]. However, a careful examination of the IGT reveals a critical confounding between EV and gain-loss frequency. On average, for each 10-card unit, deck A contains 5 gains and 5 losses, and deck B contains 9 gains and 1 loss. On the other hand, deck C contains 6.25 gains, 2.5 standoffs and 1.25 losses, and D contains 9 gains and 1 loss. Altogether the bad decks (A and B) contain 14 gains and 6 losses, whereas good decks (C and D) contain 15.25 gains, 2.5 standoffs, and only 2.25 losses. Both good and bad decks have a similar number of gains, whereas the good decks have significantly fewer losses (see Tables 1 and 2). Therefore, it is not clear whether subjects' choices of good decks in the IGT were driven by improved EV or gain-loss frequency. This study attempts to generate a symmetrical and fair experiment to demonstrate the relative guiding power of gain-loss frequency and EV under uncertainty, specifically to identify which factor most comprehensively dominates normal subject preferences. To differentiate between the relative contributions of EV and gain-loss frequency, this study applies a new task, namely, the Soochow Gambling Task (SGT) [40, 41] (see Table 1).

Table 1 The immediate net value of each trial and gain-loss structure in the original IGT and SGT
Table 2 A comparison of gamble structures between the Iowa Gambling Task and Soochow Gambling Task

Method

A two-phase study was designed to explore learning process and final preferences during decision making. Four card decks, randomized into 24 arrangements (e.g., ABCD, BCDA, CDAB, etc.), were presented on a computer screen. Forty-eight college students and adults (24 males, 24 females; age: 18–30 years, mean age: 20.71 years) participated in this study. Each subject undertook one set of card arrangement to balance the position effect. After completing the computer game, subjects answered a questionnaire that gauged their memories of task characteristics and their deck preferences.

The SGT contains four decks, each containing 5 card-turning trials as a unit, for a total of 100 trials. The "bad" decks (defined by EV, as in IGT) in the SGT have worse EV ($ -250) and better gain-loss frequency (4 gains and 1 loss), whereas the "good" decks have better EV ($ +250) and worse gain-loss frequency (1 gain and 4 losses). Thus, in the SGT, the operation of gain-loss frequency and EV variables will predict different choice patterns. If decision-making is guided by EV, subjects will prefer the good decks (C and D) over the bad decks (A and B). Conversely, when participant choice behavior is controlled by immediate gain-loss, the perceived gain will keep subjects on the same deck. The deck with the highest number of gains will increase the probability of being chosen. Table 2 summarizes a comparison of the task structures and predictions between the IGT and the SGT.

The SGT adopted an experimental procedure similar to that used in the IGT to assure that subjects perform the task under uncertain conditions. The probability structures of gain-loss frequency were unknown to subjects in both tasks (IGT and SGT). Subjects were also ignorant of the time limitation in the experiment. Both tasks then are uncertain in the traditional sense.

This study adopted the subject instructions employed by Bechara et al. [36]. The key points of the instructions are as follows:

"The goal of the game is to win as much money as possible and, if you find yourself unable to win, make sure you avoid losing money as much as possible. I won't tell you for how long the game will continue. You must keep on playing until the computer stops. It is important to know that the colors of the cards are irrelevant in this game. The computer does not make you lose money at random. However, there is no way for you to figure out when the computer will make you lose. All I can say is that you may find yourself losing money on all decks, but some decks will make lose more than others. You can win if you stay away from the worst decks ...." (Bechara et al., 1999, p. 5474).

Results

Experimental results showed that subjects preferred the bad decks (A and B) to the good decks (C and D) (Figure 1). This finding supports the effect of prediction-based gain-loss frequency and is contrary to that of EV.

Figure 1
figure 1

Mean number of cards chosen. The results of two factors (repeated measurement) ANOVA (Gain-loss frequencies (gain vs. loss) × Values (± 200 vs. ± 100)) showed a significant effect of gain-loss frequency. Subjects selected more cards from decks A and B than decks C and D (F (1, 47) = 26.41, p < .01). The value effect with of the paired t-test is significant (t (47) = 2.60, p < .05) under high-frequency gain (+200 vs. +100), but not significant (t (47) = 0.72, p = .48) under high-frequency loss (-200 vs. -100). None of the interaction effects are statistically significant (F (1, 47) = 1.88, p = .18).

Moreover, the average number of times subjects chose bad decks was higher than that for good decks throughout the entire experiment (Figure 2). Significant interaction was found between gain-loss frequency and blocks. However, no single main effect of blocks was observed. Subjects gradually adjusted their selection pattern to the chance level of four choices. The absence of a crossover or lack of learning curve indicates that gain-loss frequency was dominant over EV when subjects were choosing decks.

Figure 2
figure 2

Mean number of cards chosen in blocks. 100 card selection trials were grouped into five blocks, each comprising 20 trials. The three factor (repeated measurement) ANOVA (Gain-loss frequencies (gain vs. loss) × Values (± 200 vs. ± 100) × Blocks (1 to 5)) indicated a significant main effect for gain-loss frequencies (F (1, 47) = 29.44, p < .01) and values (F (1, 47) = 9.02, p < .01), but not for blocks (F (1, 47) = 0.00, p = 1.00). Furthermore, significant interactions existed between gain-loss frequencies and blocks (F (4, 44) = 3.03, p < .05) as well as three factors (F (4, 44) = 5.19, p < .01); but non-significant interactions existed between gain-loss frequencies and values (F (1, 47) = 1.90, p = .18); values and blocks (F (4, 44) = 0.99, p = .43). These results indicate a clear preference for the pooled decks A and B ("bad" decks) over the pooled decks C and D ("good" decks) from the beginning. Subjects seem to be guided by gain-loss frequencies and appear sensitive to the gain-loss structure gradually. No cross-over or significant learning curve exists for the high-frequency gain (A, B) and high-frequency loss (C, D) decks under this condition (100 trials) in the Soochow Gambling Task.

Furthermore, this investigation also analyzes the pattern of continuing choices after the subject chose AB or CD (Figure 3). The experimental results showed that subjects tend to remain on the same type of decks while choosing A or B. Nevertheless, no significant differences are found for the continuing choices between the same or the different types of decks while they were choosing C or D.

Figure 3
figure 3

Mean probability of shift and stay in the continuing choice. After choosing the (bad) decks A or B, 65% of participants remain on these two decks, with only 35% shifting to the (good) decks C and D (t (47) = 8.60, p < .01). On the other hand, when subjects choose decks C and D, the probabilities of them shifting or staying in their next selection was a roughly 50/50 probability of selecting deck A and B versus C and D (t (47) = 1.71, p = .09).

Analysis of the questionnaire results indicated that subjects correctly recalled which decks they chose that had the highest gains (Figure 4A); but subjects have a chance-level recollection on which decks they chose that had the highest losses (Figure 4B). Conversely, subjects incorrectly recalled which decks were in use when they won the largest overall amount of money (Figure 4C); but subjects have a chance-level to recall which decks were chosen when they lost the largest overall amount of money (Figure 4D). Preference patterns of conscious recollection were consistent with the choice pattern due to the effect of gain-loss frequency (Figures 4E and 4F).

Figure 4
figure 4

Subject memory assessment in the Soochow Gambling Task. Forty-eight subjects were required to report their behavior and preferences after completion of the game. (4A) Most of the sample (36 subjects) had vivid impressions for high-frequency gain decks (A+B) (x2 (1) = 12.00, p < .01), (4B) but not high-frequency loss (C+D) (x2 (1) = 0.00, p = 1.00). (4C) Additionally, most subjects (31 subjects recognized decks A and B) possessed a clear (but wrong) image for the overall monetary gain (x2 (1) = 4.08, p < .05); (4D) but a blurred image for overall monetary loss (x2 (1) = .08, p = .77). (4E) After completing the game, subjects erroneously equated the high-frequency gains as the overall advantage. Thirty one out of forty eight subjects indicated the favorable choice to decks A and B rather than C and D (x2 (1) = 4.08, p < .05). (4F) Most unfavorable choice they had memorized were decks C and D which possessed the high-frequency loss and positive expected value (x2 (1) = 8.33, p < .01).

Discussion

Subjects may apply an implicit strategy to cope with the uncertain game, therefore they favored high-frequency gains over high-frequency losses in the experiment. This "gain-stay, lose-randomize" strategy (Figure 3) [42] has been observed in human and animal appetitive and avoidance experiments in which human or animal encounter reward or punishment [42–48]. These pioneer behavior studies with the concurrent schedules of reinforcement have displayed the frequency effect for choice pattern [45–49]. Additionally, these concepts have also been applied to examine the behavioral model of neuropsychological deficit [50, 51].

The SGT possessed the variant frequency, magnitude and delay of reinforcement/punishment depending on each subject's choice. Most part of these set-ups may be similar to the consideration of the traditional behavior-analysis studies [45–49, 52], therefore, the gain-loss frequency can successfully serve as a predictor for choice behavior under these uncertain situations.

The effect of gain-loss frequency is not an isolated finding in similar settings. A reexamination of the bad decks (A and B) in the original IGT [8] indicated that bad deck B (9 gains and 1 loss) was also chosen more frequently than deck A (5 gains and 5 losses). Other studies obtained similar findings but did not explore them further [18–28, 33, 34]. Furthermore, some research groups even showed that normal subjects chose the disadvantageous deck B more frequently than the advantageous deck C or D [18–28]. Dunn et al. [14] sampled 38 IGT related studies to demonstrate that only five studies [18, 19, 23, 25, 53] utilized the "four-deck format" to display their findings (i.e., the number of card turnings for each deck over a total of 100 trials being shown separately). These studies all demonstrated that deck B was chosen more frequently than deck A. It is worth noting that four out of five studies [18, 19, 23, 25, 53] also demonstrated that deck B was chosen more than deck C or D.

Furthermore, in Peters and Slovic study [54], the modified IGT study also demonstrated that deck B ($ -250) and D ($ +250) possessed the inversed expected values, but with nearly equal attraction to subjects. This may imply that the expected value does not guide decision makers to approach the beneficial choice in these dynamic games. On the other hand, in the modified IGT, deck C contained high-frequency gain (8 out of 10 trials) over deck D (5 out of 10 trials), but with nearly equal expected value ($ +300 for C vs. $ +250 for D). However, subjects preferred deck C rather than deck D significantly. The gain-loss frequency seems to be more reasonable than EV in explaining these observations. Ahn et al. [55] confirmed the present finding by comparing the decision learning models for IGT and SGT respectively.

Questionnaire data in this study also indicate a novel phenomenon, namely, the "money account illusion". Subjects ignored the EV dimension and miscounted the money amount in terms of the strength of frequency, specifically, frequently receiving gains will leave an overall impression of large accumulated monetary outcome for a deck than when gains are infrequently received.

In the IGT, normal subjects shifted their choices to good decks during the latter part of the game, such that a learning curve was evident [7–9, 36, 37, 39]. We propose that this shift is not due to better EV but rather an effect resulting from more frequent gains than losses. The Somatic Marker Hypothesis stresses that somatic markers (or peripheral feedback) predispose normal subjects to behave in accordance with perceived future consequences over the long run. This study demonstrated that even subjects with intact somatic markers cannot behave accordingly to a search for EV in the SGT. Immediate reinforcement will override EV in the SGT. Decision makers may have been guided by immediate gain and, as such, their behavioral results are consistent with the prediction of gain-loss frequency. Based on the present observation, three possible interpretations exist for SMH: 1) somatic marker system may guide decision making behavior via rough-estimation processing (gain-loss frequency), not a precise calculation (EV: probability × value); 2) somatic marker system may only contribute to generating subjective feelings (consciousness), and may not be immediately related to decision guidance; 3) the operation of the somatic marker may be involved in gathering the long-term memory, but may not globally direct choice behavior in situations of high uncertainty.

The Somatic Marker Hypothesis also posits that somatic markers guide advantageous behavior in a non-conscious manner. However, questionnaire results suggest further that subjects can have a clear knowledge of gain and loss frequency by the end of the game. This perception eventually determines their choice patterns. A similar finding was obtained by Maia and McClelland [34], demonstrating a possible "conscious" knowledge of EV in the IGT.

In the SGT, normal subjects were stuck with the influence of gain-loss frequency without shifting to EV throughout the entire session. If normal subjects cannot resist the influence of gain-loss frequency, ventromedial prefrontal patients would have increased vulnerability to the effect of immediate gains and losses. This prediction seems to be in line with disinhibition theory that suggests that socially dysfunctional patients with prefrontal damage have difficulty avoiding a punishment-associated stimulus when that stimulus was previously associated with a reward [13, 15, 16, 56]. If this is true, then both normal subjects and ventromedial prefrontal patients will not be responsive to the long-term dimension.

Supposing this is the case, the facilitative effect of somatic markers did not induce a learning effect or an advantageous shifting behavior in the long run in the SGT, as consistently proposed by IGT researchers. Careful analysis of experimental results obtained in this study identified a subordinate phenomenon in that deck A was chosen more often than deck B, despite both decks possessing the same gain-loss frequency. The immediate value of gain and loss can also alter slightly decision-makers' choices and is worthy of further exploration.

EV and gain-loss frequency were seriously confounded in the administration of the IGT. Selection of good decks by normal subjects cannot only be attributed to the effect of EV, but must also be explained in terms of gain-loss frequency. The Somatic Marker Hypothesis was further reinforced by adopting the principal findings from the IGT experiment as important supporting data, i.e., somatic markers predispose normal subjects to search for EV in the long run. This study explored separately the relative contribution of EV and gain-loss frequency in the SGT, a modified gambling task. Experimental results indicated that immediate reinforcement overrides the effect of EV. Crone et al. [57] also identified a local preference for gain-loss frequency; however, EV dominated gain-loss frequency in their modified IGT. In contrast to the predictions based on Somatic Marker Hypothesis, experimental data in this study indicate that normal subjects were primarily guided by the effect of gain-loss frequency rather than EV. This finding, although differing from the simple explanation provided by the Somatic Marker Hypothesis, is consistent with the view of behavioral [1–5] and affective [5, 20, 58–62] decision literature, indicating that normal individuals are often short-sighted when making decisions in stock market or real life [41, 63, 64].

Conclusion

A serious confounding effect in IGT was demonstrated by the "prominent deck B phenomenon". This recently-discovered phenomenon may imply that gain-loss frequency rather than EV dominates the choice behavior of normal decision makers under uncertainty. In the original IGT, these two factors were seriously confounded, and thus this study proposed a modified task SGT to clarify the relative contribution of these two factors. The experimental results indicated that subject selection patterns were mostly predicted based on gain-loss frequency. Immediate gain may increase the probability of continuing to stick on the chosen deck, which is consistent with most behavioral-analysis and decision literatures. The observations of this study indicate that EV is not predictive for choice behaviors as suggested by IGT. It seems that immediate gain eventually resulted in a long-term loss. The basic assumption of IGT is thus no longer plausible to assure the foresighted decision makers in the Iowa Gambling Task.

Abbreviations

EV:

Expected Value

IGT:

Iowa Gambling Task

SGT:

Soochow Gambling Task

SMH:

Somatic Marker Hypothesis

References

  1. Hastie R, Dawes RM: Rational Decision in an uncertainty world: The psychology of judgment and decision making. 2001, London: Sage Publications

    Google Scholar 

  2. Kahneman D: Maps of bounded rationality: Psychology for behavioral economics. Amer Econ Rev. 2003, 93: 1449-1475. 10.1257/000282803322655392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Kahneman D, Tversky A: Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. 1979, 47: 263-291. 10.2307/1914185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Tversky A, Kahneman D: The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science. 1981, 211: 453-458. 10.1126/science.7455683.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Panksepp J: Damasio's Error. Consciousness and Emotion. 2003, 4: 111-134. 10.1075/ce.4.1.10pan.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Lichtenstein S, Slovic P, Zink DJ: Effect of instruction in expected value on optimality of gambling decisions. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1969, 79: 236-240. 10.1037/h0026888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Damasio AR: Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. 1994, New York: G. P. Putnam's Sons

    Google Scholar 

  8. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW: Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994, 50: 7-15. 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR: Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997, 275: 1293-1295. 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Leland J, Grafman J: Experimental tests of Somatic Marker hypothesis. Games Econ Behav. 2005, 52: 386-409. 10.1016/j.geb.2004.09.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Tomb I, Hauser M, Deldin P, Caramazza A: Do somatic markers mediate decisions on the gambling task?. Nat Neurosci. 2002, 5: 1103-1104. 10.1038/nn1102-1103.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Suzuki A, Hirota A, Takasawa N, Shigemasu K: Application of the somatic marker hypothesis to individual differences in decision making. Biol Psychol. 2003, 65 (1): 81-88. 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00093-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Rolls ET: Emotion Explained. 2005, New York: Oxford University Press

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  14. Dunn BD, Dalgleish T, Lawrence AD: The somatic marker hypothesis: A critical evaluation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006, 30 (2): 239-271. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Rolls ET: The brain and emotion. 1999, New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  16. Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET: The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Prog Neurobiol. 2004, 72: 341-372. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.03.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Lin CH, Chiu YC, Huang JT: Growing evidence of prominent deck B phenomenon in Iowa Gambling Task. Society of Neuroeconomics 5th Annual Meeting: 2007; Boston, MA. 2007

    Google Scholar 

  18. Bark R, Dieckmann S, Bogerts B, Northoff G: Deficit in decision making in catatonic schizophrenia: an exploratory study. Psychiatry Res. 2005, 134 (2): 131-141. 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.04.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Wilder KE, Weinberger DR, Goldberg TE: Operant conditioning and the orbitofrontal cortex in schizophrenic patients: unexpected evidence for intact functioning. Schizophr Res. 1998, 30 (2): 169-174. 10.1016/S0920-9964(97)00135-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. O'Carroll RE, Papps BP: Decision making in humans: the effect of manipulating the central noradrenergic system. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2003, 74 (3): 376-378. 10.1136/jnnp.74.3.376.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Crone EA, Somsen RJM, van Beek B, van Der Molen MW: Heart rate and skin conductance analysis of antecendents and consequences of decision making. Psychophysiology. 2004, 41: 531-540. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00197.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Overman WH, Frassrand K, Ansel S, Trawalter S, Bies B, Redmond A: Performance on the IOWA card task by adolescents and adults. Neuropsychologia. 2004, 42 (13): 1838-1851. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.03.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Ritter LM, Meador-Woodruff JH, Dalack GW: Neurocognitive measures of prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004, 68 (1): 65-73. 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00086-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Rodriguez-Sanchez JM, Crespo-Facorro B, Perez-Iglesias R, Gonzalez-Blanch C, Alvarez-Jimenez M, Llorca J, Vazquez-Barquero JL: Prefrontal cognitive functions in stabilized first-episode patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a dissociation between dorsolateral and orbitofrontal functioning. Schizophr Res. 2005, 77 (2–3): 279-288. 10.1016/j.schres.2005.04.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Toplak ME, Jain U, Tannock R: Executive and motivational processes in adolescents with Attention-Deficit-Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). Behav Brain Funct. 2005, 1 (1): 8-10.1186/1744-9081-1-8.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Fernie G, Tunney RJ: Some decks are better than others: the effect of reinforcer type and task instructions on learning in the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain Cogn. 2006, 60 (1): 94-102. 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.09.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Caroselli JS, Hiscock M, Scheibel RS, Ingram F: The simulated gambling paradigm applied to young adults: an examination of university students' performance. Appl Neuropsychol. 2006, 13 (4): 203-212. 10.1207/s15324826an1304_1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Martino DJ, Bucay D, Butman JT, Allegri RF: Neuropsychological frontal impairments and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2007

    Google Scholar 

  29. Lin CH, Chiu YC, Lee PL, Hsieh JC: Is deck B a disadvantageous deck in the Iowa Gambling Task?. Behav Brain Funct. 2007, 3: 16-10.1186/1744-9081-3-16.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Sevy S, Burdick KE, Visweswaraiah H, Abdelmessih S, Lukin M, Yechiam E, Bechara A: Iowa gambling task in schizophrenia: a review and new data in patients with schizophrenia and co-occurring cannabis use disorders. Schizophr Res. 2007, 92 (1–3): 74-84. 10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.005.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Johnson CA, Xiao L, Palmer P, Sun P, Wang Q, Wei Y, Jia Y, Grenard JL, Stacy AW, Bechara A: Affective decision-making deficits, linked to a dysfunctional ventromedial prefrontal cortex, revealed in 10th grade Chinese adolescent binge drinkers. Neuropsychologia. 2008, 46 (2): 714-726. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.09.012.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Chiu YC, Lin CH: Is deck C an advantageous deck in the Iowa Gambling Task?. Behav Brain Funct. 2007, 3 (37):

  33. Lin CH, Chiu YC, Lee PL, Hsieh JC: The preferences of decision-making in IOWA Gambling Task: The testing of frequency effect and long-term outcomes in Somatic Marker Hypothesis. 2nd Conference of NeuroEconomics: 2004; Münster, Germany. 2004

    Google Scholar 

  34. Maia TV, McClelland JL: A reexamination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: what participants really know in the Iowa gambling task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004, 101 (45): 16075-16080. 10.1073/pnas.0406666101.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Bowman CH, Evans CEY, Turnbull OH: Artificial time constraints on the Iowa Gambling Task: The effects on behavioral performance and subjective experience. Brain Cogn. 2005, 57: 21-25. 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Lee GP: Different contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J Neurosci. 1999, 19: 5473-5481.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR: Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1996, 6: 215-225. 10.1093/cercor/6.2.215.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Anderson SW: Dissociation Of working memory from decision making within the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 1998, 18 (1): 428-437.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR: Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000, 123: 2189-2202. 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Chiu YC, Lin CH, Huang JT, Lin S, Lee PL, Hsieh JC: Immediate gain is long-term loss: Are there foresighted decision makers in Iowa gambling task?. Society for Neuroeconomics 3rd Annual Meeting: September 15–18 2005; Kiawah Island, South Carolina, U.S.A. 2005

    Google Scholar 

  41. Chiu YC, Lin CH, Lin S, Huang JT: Reexamining the Effect of Long-term Outcome and Gain-loss Frequency: From Uncertainty to Certainty. Society for Neuroeconomics 4th Annual Meeting: 2006; Park City, Utah, U.S.A. 2006

    Google Scholar 

  42. Mitropoulos A: An experiment on the value of structural information in a 2 × 2 repeated game. Econ Letters. 2003, 78: 27-32. 10.1016/S0165-1765(02)00174-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Colman AM: Oxford Dictionary of Psychology. 2003, New York: Oxford University Press, 1

    Google Scholar 

  44. Herrnstein RJ: Formal properties of the matching law. J Exp Anal Behav. 1974, 21: 159-164. 10.1901/jeab.1974.21-159.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Rachlin H: Introduction to modern behaviorism. 1991, New York: W. H. Freeman and Company, 3

    Google Scholar 

  46. Schwartz B, Wasserman EA, Robbins SJ: Psychology of learning and Behavior. 2002, New York: W. W. Norton & Company, 5

    Google Scholar 

  47. Catania AC, Sagvolden T: Preference for free choice over forced choice in pigeons. J Exp Anal Behav. 1980, 34: 77-86. 10.1901/jeab.1980.34-77.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Catania AC: Learning. 1998, NJ: Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, 4

    Google Scholar 

  49. Domjan M: The priciples of learning and behavior. 2002, Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thompson, fourth

    Google Scholar 

  50. Sagvolden T, Aase H, Zeiner P, Berger D: Altered reinforcement mechanisms in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behav Brain Res. 1998, 94 (1): 61-71. 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)00170-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Johansen EB, Killeen PR, Sagvolden T: Behavioral variability, elimination of responses, and delay-of-reinforcement gradients in SHR and WKY rats. Behav Brain Funct. 2007, 3: 60-10.1186/1744-9081-3-60.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Ferster CB, Skinner BF: Schedules of reinforcement. 1957, MA: Prentice-Hall, Inc

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  53. Shurman B, Horan WP, Nuechterlein KH: Schizophrenia patients demonstrate a distinctive pattern of decision-making impairment on the Iowa Gambling Task. Schizophr Res. 2005, 72 (2–3): 215-224. 10.1016/j.schres.2004.03.020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Peters E, Slovic P: The springs of action: Affective and analytical Information processing in choice. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2000, 26: 1465-1475. 10.1177/01461672002612002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Ahn WY, Busemeyer JR, Wagenmakers EJ, Stout JC: Comparison of decision learning models using the generalization criterion method. Cogn Sci. 2008, Accepted pending revisions,

    Google Scholar 

  56. Rolls ET, Hornak J, Wade D, McGrath J: Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994, 57: 1518-1524.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Crone EA, van der Molen MW: Developmental changes in real life decision making: performance on a gambling task previously shown to depend on the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Dev Neuropsychol. 2004, 25 (3): 251-279. 10.1207/s15326942dn2503_2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Berridge K: Irrational Pursuits: Hyper-Incentives from a Visceral Brain. The psychology of economics decisions: Volume I: Rationality and Well-Being. Edited by: Brocas I, Carrillo J. 2003, New York: Oxford University Press, 17-40.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Baumeister R: The psychology of irrationality: Why people make foolish, self-defeating choices. The psychology of economics decisions: Volume I: Rationality and Well-Being. Edited by: Brocas I, Carrillo J. 2003, New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  60. Heims HC, Critchley HD, Dolan R, Mathias CJ, Cipolotti L: Social and motivational functioning is not critically dependent on feedback of autonomic responses: neuropsychological evidence from patients with pure autonomic failure. Neuropsychologia. 2004, 42 (14): 1979-1988. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. North NT, O'Carroll RE: Decision making in patients with spinal cord damage: afferent feedback and the somatic marker hypothesis. Neuropsychologia. 2001, 39 (5): 521-524. 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00107-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Ross D, Spurrett D, Vuchinich R: The behavioral economics and neuroeconomics disordered gambling: A policy-focused survey of research. A report prepared for the South African Responsible Gambling Foundation. Cape Town. 2006, 1-255.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Peterson R: Inside the Investor's Brain: The Power of Mind Over Money. 2007, SA: John Wiley & Sons

    Google Scholar 

  64. Lin CH, Chiu YC, Huang JT: Is decision-maker sensitive to expected value in the dynamic-uncertain gambles?. 5th Annual Meeting of Society for Neuroeconomics: 2007; Boston, MA. 2007

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the National Science Council of the Republic of China, Taiwan, for financially supporting this research under Contract No. NSC 94-2413-H031-002. Professors Keng-Chen Liang and Daisy Hung are appreciated for their valuable courses for introduction of Somatic Marker Hypothesis. Dr. Ian Tomb and Professor Marc Hauser are also appreciated for kindly providing the original instruction of IGT. Thanks also extend to the editors and reviewers of several journals over the past 4 years for their valuable comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Ching-Hung Lin or Jong-Tsun Huang.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors certify that the information listed above is complete to the best of our original research. The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

This design of Soochow Gambling Task was constructed by YC and CH, both have made equal contributions to thought, data interpretation, drafting the key concept, and revising it critically. JT provided several times of critical review to organize the whole picture for this work and revised this article significantly. PL worked on the computerization of task, consulting of data analysis. SY and JC participated in some discussion and provided some valuable viewpoints to improve the meaning of data and readability. All authors gave final approval of the version to be published.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chiu, YC., Lin, CH., Huang, JT. et al. Immediate gain is long-term loss: Are there foresighted decision makers in the Iowa Gambling Task?. Behav Brain Funct 4, 13 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-4-13

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-4-13

Keywords