Skip to main content

Mental fatigue caused by prolonged cognitive load associated with sympathetic hyperactivity

Abstract

Background

It is known that chronic fatigue is associated with sympathetic hyperactivity. However, the relationship between autonomic function and mental fatigue caused by a prolonged mental load in healthy humans is still unclear. Thus, in order to clarify the mechanisms underlying mental fatigue, we examined the association between mental fatigue and autonomic functions.

Methods

The study group comprised 10 healthy participants. To induce mental fatigue, participants performed mental tasks, which consisted of the advanced trail making test, kana pick-out test and mirror drawing test, for 8 hr, corresponding to a normal work day. Autonomic functions were measured by accelerated plethysmography before and after the fatigue-inducing mental tasks. As a control, the same participants completed an 8-hr relaxation session 4 weeks before the fatigue session.

Results

After the 8-hr relaxation session, low-frequency component power (LF), high-frequency component power (HF) and low-frequency component power/high-frequency component power ratio (LF/HF ratio) were not changed from baseline. In contrast, after the fatigue session, the HF and LF/HF ratio were significantly changed from baseline; specifically, the HF was lower and LF/HF ratio was higher as compared to those after the relaxation session.

Conclusions

Sympathetic hyperactivity based on decreased parasympathetic activity is associated with mental fatigue induced by prolonged cognitive load.

Background

Fatigue, best defined as the difficulty in initiating or sustaining voluntary activities [1], is a common symptom of various illnesses, and can even be observed in healthy individuals [2–4]. Acute fatigue is a normal phenomenon that disappears after a period of rest. In contrast, long-term fatigue (chronic fatigue) is sometimes irreversible and the compensation mechanisms that are useful in reducing acute fatigue are no longer effective. Chronic fatigue is caused by the prolonged accumulation of acute fatigue. Thus, in order to avoid chronic fatigue, it is important to develop effective strategies to recover from and avoid the accumulation of acute fatigue.

Alterations of autonomic functions have been reported in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS) [5–10], multiple sclerosis (MS) [11–13], and primary biliary cirrhosis (PBC) [8, 14]. In addition, autonomic dysfunction is associated with fatigue in patients with CFS [8], MS [11, 12], and PBC [8, 14]. These reports suggest that autonomic activities are related to the mechanisms underlying fatigue. However, this association has been demonstrated only in patients with specific diseases, not in healthy volunteers.

Fatigue is classified into physical and mental types. Physical fatigue, also known as peripheral fatigue, results from repeated muscle actions. In contrast, mental fatigue represents a failure to complete mental tasks that require self-motivation and internal cues in the absence of demonstrable cognitive failure or motor weakness [15]. Thus, mental fatigue decreases sufferers' work or study efficiency in daily life.

Recently, we demonstrated that decreased parasympathetic activity and increased sympathetic activity were induced in healthy volunteers following a 30-min series of fatigue-inducing mental tasks [16]. These results suggested that, as motivation has been reported to be associated with increased sympathetic activity [17, 18], motivation against potential impairment of task performance caused by mental fatigue may contribute to the increased sympathetic activity observed during fatigue-inducing mental tasks. This finding clarified the relationship between mental fatigue and autonomic functions during fatigue-inducing mental tasks. However, the relationship between mental fatigue and autonomic functions during rest after the completion of a fatigue-inducing mental task remains unclear. Because deterioration of autonomic functions in patients with CFS, a human model of severe fatigue [19], is observed during rest, a difference in autonomic functions between chronic and acute fatigue might also be identified in healthy volunteers by evaluating autonomic functions during rest after a fatigue-inducing mental task. If the alternative pattern of autonomic functions is similar between CFS patients and fatigued healthy volunteers, the measurement of autonomic functions is useful as a common objective physiological marker for chronic and acute fatigue, as well as for evaluation of the effects of intervention against acute fatigue in order to prevent chronic fatigue. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to clarify the relationship between autonomic activity and mental fatigue after 8 hr of fatigue-inducing mental tasks, corresponding to a normal work day for most healthy adults.

Methods

Participants

The study group comprised 10 healthy volunteers [age, 27.6 ± 4.9 years (mean ± SD); 4 women, and 6 men]. Recruitment condition was decided based on previous studies [20–25]. The following potential study participants were excluded from the study: those who had a history of health problems; those taking chronic medication or supplemental vitamins; current smokers; those who weighed <40 kg; those who had donated blood within 1 month before the study; and those with a blood hemoglobin level <12.0 g/dl. Good health was also assessed by physical examination, blood pressure, heart rate, blood chemistry panel (glucose, creatinine, uremic nitrogen, sodium, potassium, chloride, uric acid, and creatine phosphokinase), lipid profile (total cholesterol and triglyceride), complete blood count, and urinalysis. Participants who manifested psychiatric morbidity (e.g., depression) were excluded from the study. Psychiatric evaluation and diagnosis were made by a psychiatrist (O. K.). The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Kansai University of Welfare Sciences and all participants provided written informed consent.

Experimental design

All participants underwent both fatigue and relaxation sessions. The fatigue session was performed 4 weeks after the relaxation session in order to eliminate the effects of menstrual cycle for female participants. The day before either type of session, participants were instructed to avoid intensive physical and mental activities, to finish dinner by 10:00 p.m., and to fast overnight. At 9:00 a.m. the following morning, participants recorded their subjective sensation of fatigue on a visual analogue scale (VAS) from 0 (no fatigue) to 100 (complete exhaustion), and autonomic function was measured using accelerated plethysmography (APG). Participants then ate breakfast (carbohydrate, 73.6 g; protein, 26.9 g; lipid, 32.3 g; total calories, 707 kcal) between 9:45 and 10:15 a.m. After breakfast, either a relaxation or fatigue session was performed. During the fatigue sessions, participants performed an advanced trail making test [26, 27] for 45 min, a kana pick-out test [28] for 30 min, and a mirror drawing test [29] for 45 min. Beginning at 10:15 a.m., four rotations of this series of tasks were performed for a total of 8 hr of mental fatigue-inducing activities. After the end of the second rotation, between 3:15 and 3:45 p.m., the participants ate lunch (carbohydrate, 124.2 g; protein, 26.4 g; lipid, 16.3 g; total calories, 844 kcal). The time interval between the first and second rotations and between the third and fourth rotations was 30 min. Upon completion of the fourth rotation, between 8:15 and 8:45 p.m., participants again recorded their subjective sensation of fatigue on the VAS, and autonomic functions were recorded using APG. During the relaxation sessions, participants read books, watched movies or talked among themselves for the same time frame used for the fatigue sessions.

Fatigue-inducing mental tasks

The fatigue-inducing mental tasks used in the present study were chosen based on previous reports [21, 22, 25, 27]. The advanced trail making test was an advanced version of the conventional trail making test [30]. In the test, circles numbered from 1 to 25 were randomly located on the display of a personal computer. Participants were required to touch the circles in sequence, starting with number 1. When participants clicked on the first target circle using the computer mouse, the circle disappeared and circle number 26 appeared in a different position on the screen. The positions of the other circles remained the same. Participants were required to memorize the positions of the other circles while searching for the target circle. Participants were instructed to complete the test as quickly and as accurately as possible.

In the kana pick-out test, participants were instructed to identify as many vowels as possible while at the same time understanding the meaning of the story in a Japanese-language novel. Every 8 min after the start of the test, participants were asked questions about the contents of the novel for 2 min in order to assess participants' reading comprehension.

In the mirror drawing test, participants traced a character on a small glass mirror, which reversed the image. Participants were instructed to perform this test as quickly and as accurately as possible.

APG

Accelerated plethysmography has been used for the evaluation of autonomic functions [31–34]. In the present study, APG was performed using a pulsimeter (Artett, U-Medica, Osaka, Japan) with the sensor positioned on the tip of the ventral side of the index finger. Photoplethysmography was used to measure changes in the absorption of light by hemoglobin, which is related to blood flow volume. The pulsimeter performed automatic analyses of the second derivative of the photoplethysmographic waveform, which is known as the APG waveform. Participants underwent APG sitting quietly with their eyes closed for 2 min. The APG waveform consists of four waves in systole (a - d) and one in diastole (e). The sensor output of the pulsimeter was preprocessed by an analogue filter (2nd order, low pass filter with 23 Hz of cut off frequency). The data were recorded using an analogue-to-digital converter (3.3 volt to 10 bit) and a real-time sampling rate of 1,000 samples per second. These digital data were processed with a 67th order finite impulse response filter using the Hanning window. Detected peak times were interpolated to sub-millisecond order. Frequency analyses for pulse-interval variation were analyzed with fast Fourier transform. Resolution ability for power spectrum was 0.001 Hz. For the frequency analyses, the total power was calculated as the power within a frequency range of 0 - 0.4 Hz, the low-frequency component power (LF) was calculated as the power within a frequency range of 0.04 - 0.15 Hz, and the high-frequency component power (HF) was calculated as that within a frequency range of 0.15 - 0.4 Hz. The HF is vagally mediated [35–37], whereas the LF originates from a variety of sympathetic and vagal mechanisms [35, 38]. The low-frequency component power/high-frequency component power ratio (LF/HF ratio) is considered to represent sympathetic activity [39].

Statistical analyses

The VAS values for fatigue and pulse rates are shown as the mean ± SD. When statistically significant effects for session (relaxation and fatigue) or time course (baseline and 8 hr) and/or session × time course interactions in each value were found by two-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), and a paired t-test was used to evaluate the significance of differences between the relaxation and fatigue sessions or between baseline and 8 hr for each session. A Wilcoxon signed-rank test was used to evaluate the significance of differences in each spectral component between the relaxation and fatigue sessions or between baseline and 8 hr for each session. Spearman's and Pearson's correlation analyses between the VAS value for fatigue and LF/HF ratio and log-transformed LF/HF ratio (ln LF/HF ratio) were performed, respectively. All p values were 2-tailed, and p values <.05 were considered statistically significant. Statistical analyses were performed using the SPSS 17.0 software package (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).

Results

Visual analogue scale values for fatigue and pulse rates before and after the relaxation and fatigue sessions are shown in Table 1. Although two-way repeated measures ANOVA of the VAS values for fatigue revealed no significant effect of session (F(1,9) = 2.03, p = .188), it revealed a significant effect of time course (F(1,9) = 7.50, p = .023) and session × time course interactions (F(1,9) = 12.74, p = .006). The VAS values for fatigue before the experiment (baseline) did not differ between the relaxation and fatigue sessions. The VAS value for fatigue was increased from baseline after the fatigue session, and the VAS value for fatigue after the fatigue session was greater than that after the relaxation session, indicating that the series of mental tasks performed during the fatigue session induced fatigue in the participants. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA of pulse rates revealed no significant effect of session (F(1,9) = 3.10, p = .112), time course (F(1,9) = 3.60, p = .090) or session × time course interactions (F(1,9) = 2.28, p = .165).

Table 1 Visual analogue scale (VAS) value for fatigue and autonomic activities before (Baseline) and after (8 hr) the relaxation and fatigue sessions

Autonomic functions before and after the relaxation and fatigue sessions are shown in Table 1. The total power at baseline did not differ between the relaxation and fatigue sessions. Although the total power was not altered from the baseline after the relaxation session, the total power was decreased from the baseline after the fatigue session. Low-frequency component power between the relaxation and fatigue sessions or between the baseline and 8 hr for each session were similar. High-frequency component power at baseline did not differ between the relaxation and fatigue sessions. The HF was decreased from the baseline after the fatigue session, and the HF after the fatigue session was lower than that after the relaxation session. Although the LF/HF ratio at baseline did not differ between the relaxation and fatigue sessions, the LF/HF ratio was increased from baseline after the fatigue session, and the LF/HF ratio after the fatigue session was higher than that after the relaxation session.

Correlation analyses between the VAS value for fatigue and the LF/HF ratio or the ln LF/HF ratio in all the measurement points which are before and after the 8-hr fatigue and relaxation sessions are shown in Figure 1. The VAS values for fatigue were positively correlated with the LF/HF ratio (Figure 1a) and the ln LF/HF ratio (Figure 1b).

Figure 1
figure 1

Correlations between visual analogue scale (VAS) value for fatigue and autonomic activities. Low-frequency component power/high-frequency component power ratio (LF/HF ratio; a) log-transformed LF/HF ratio (ln LF/HF ratio; b) obtained on a wave interval analyses using accelerated plethysmography in all the measurement points before and after the 8-hr fatigue and relaxation sessions. Spearman's (a) and Pearson's (b) correlation coefficients and p-values are shown.

Discussion

The present study showed that, whereas the HF and the LF/HF ratio did not change from baseline after the relaxation session, these values changed significantly after the fatigue session. After the 8-hr fatigue session, the HF was lower and the LF/HF ratio was higher than those after the 8-hr relaxation session. In addition, levels of fatigue sensation were associated with the LF/HF ratio.

The present results suggest that parasympathetic activity was reduced by mental fatigue. Previous studies have consistently demonstrated a decrease in parasympathetic activity due to mental load [40–45]. These findings suggest that mental load causes reduced parasympathetic activity, and that prolonged fatigue-inducing mental load reduces parasympathetic activity even further. Thus, reduced parasympathetic activity seems to be a characteristic feature of mental fatigue.

Recently, we demonstrated that not only decreased parasympathetic activity and but also increased sympathetic activity were induced in healthy volunteers following a 30-min series of fatigue-inducing mental tasks [16], suggesting that, because motivation has been reported to be associated with increased sympathetic activity [17, 18], motivation against potential impairment of task performance caused by mental fatigue may contribute to the increased sympathetic activity observed during fatigue-inducing mental tasks. However, in the present study, sympathetic activity based on decreased parasympathetic activity was also observed despite the allotment of a rest period after the fatigue-inducing mental tasks. These results suggest that decreased parasympathetic activity and increased sympathetic activity induced by mental fatigue are associated with not only motivation but also other components.

Decreased parasympathetic activity and increased sympathetic activity are interpreted as a state of autonomic hypervigilance [46, 47]. In order to enhance parasympathetic responses for vegetative and restorative functions, and more generally under conditions of normal modern life, sympathoexcitatory response needs to be tonically inhibited [46]. Accordingly, sympathoexcitatory subcortical threat circuits are normally under the inhibitory control of the prefrontal cortex [46, 48, 49]. Several studies have reported that during prolonged mental tasks lasting 1 - 2 hr, activation of the brain regions relating to mental task processing were gradually reduced [50, 51]. During the fatigue-inducing mental tasks in the present study, the prefrontal cortex, which is associated with the processing of executive functions such as visuospatial working memory and divided attention in the advanced trail making test and kana pick-out test, respectively, may have been continuously activated. Therefore, although further neuroimaging studies are necessary, prolonged cognitive load may induce decreases in prefrontal cortex activity and inhibitory capacity for sympathoexcitatory response.

Consistent with our results, decreased parasympathetic activity and increased relative sympathetic activity have been observed in CFS patients [9, 10, 31]. Evidence such as lowered cerebral activity in the prefrontal cortex during fatigue-inducing tasks [52] and a bilateral reduction of grey-matter volume in the prefrontal cortices [53] suggests that individuals with CFS may exhibit anatomical and functional alterations in the prefrontal cortex. Since the role of the prefrontal cortex is essential in active tonic inhibition of sympathoexcitatory threat circuits, such alterations in the prefrontal cortex seen in CFS patients could be expected to lead to a decrease in parasympathetic drive, defaulting to a sympathetically driven system. Therefore, it is possible that an accumulation of mental fatigue in healthy people induces a prolonged deterioration of autonomic activity through anatomical and functional alterations of the prefrontal cortex. In order to prevent healthy individuals from suffering from chronic fatigue, early intervention and evaluation of the effect of intervention are very important. For this purpose, autonomic functions may be useful as objective physiological markers for acute and chronic fatigue.

Limitations

The present study has limitations. The standard guidelines for analyses of heart rate variability noted that LF and HF spectral components are distinguished in a spectrum calculated from short-term recordings of 2 to 5 min [54] and the study in this field has been performed based on the standard guidelines [55]. In the present study, since the APG was recorded for 2 min, the length of recording time was insufficient and the effect of breathing was not excluded adequately [54]. The number of participants tested in the present study was limited. In order to generalize our results, studies involving a larger number of participants with recordings over longer time intervals are essential.

Conclusions

The present results provide evidence that decreased parasympathetic activity and increased relative sympathetic activity are associated with mental fatigue induced by prolonged cognitive load in healthy adults. Consistent with the present findings, alterations of autonomic functions, such as decreased parasympathetic activity and increased relative sympathetic activity, have been reported in patients with CFS [9, 10, 31]. Therefore, it is possible that mental fatigue can be induced by daily events in healthy people and may eventually progress to chronic fatigue. In contrast to acute fatigue, which is a physiological phenomenon that disappears after a period of rest, chronic fatigue is sometimes irreversible, and the compensation mechanisms useful in reducing acute fatigue are not effective for chronic fatigue [56]. Thus, it is important to identify the risk factors for chronic fatigue. Our findings provide new perspectives on the mechanisms underlying acute and chronic fatigue.

Abbreviations

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

APG:

Accelerated plethysmography

CFS:

Chronic fatigue syndrome

HF:

High-frequency component power

LF:

Low-frequency component power

LF/HF ratio:

Low-frequency component power/high-frequency component power ratio

ln LF/HF ratio:

Log-transformed LF/HF ratio

MS:

Multiple sclerosis

PBC:

Primary biliary cirrhosis

SD:

Standard deviation

VAS:

Visual analogue scale.

References

  1. Chaudhuri A, Behan PO: Fatigue in neurological disorders. Lancet. 2004, 363 (9413): 978-988. 10.1016/S0140-6736(04)15794-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Chen MK: The epidemiology of self-perceived fatigue among adults. Prev Med. 1986, 15 (1): 74-81. 10.1016/0091-7435(86)90037-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Pawlikowska T, Chalder T, Hirsch SR, Wallace P, Wright DJ, Wessely SC: Population based study of fatigue and psychological distress. BMJ. 1994, 308 (6931): 763-766.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Watanabe Y: Preface and mini-review: fatigue science for human health. Fatigue Science for Human Health. Edited by: Watanabe Y, Evengård B, Natelson BH, Jason LA, Kuratsune H. 2008, New York: Springer, 5-11.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Stewart JM: Autonomic nervous system dysfunction in adolescents with postural orthostatic tachycardia syndrome and chronic fatigue syndrome is characterized by attenuated vagal baroreflex and potentiated sympathetic vasomotion. Pediatr Res. 2000, 48 (2): 218-226. 10.1203/00006450-200008000-00016.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Freeman R: The chronic fatigue syndrome is a disease of the autonomic nervous system. Sometimes. Clin Auton Res. 2002, 12 (4): 231-233. 10.1007/s10286-002-0058-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Winkler AS, Blair D, Marsden JT, Peters TJ, Wessely S, Cleare AJ: Autonomic function and serum erythropoietin levels in chronic fatigue syndrome. J Psychosom Res. 2004, 56 (2): 179-183. 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00543-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Newton JL, Okonkwo O, Sutcliffe K, Seth A, Shin J, Jones DE: Symptoms of autonomic dysfunction in chronic fatigue syndrome. QJM. 2007, 100 (8): 519-526. 10.1093/qjmed/hcm057.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Wyller VB, Saul JP, Amlie JP, Thaulow E: Sympathetic predominance of cardiovascular regulation during mild orthostatic stress in adolescents with chronic fatigue. Clin Physiol Funct Imaging. 2007, 27 (4): 231-238. 10.1111/j.1475-097X.2007.00743.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Burton AR, Rahman K, Kadota Y, Lloyd A, Vollmer-Conna U: Reduced heart rate variability predicts poor sleep quality in a case-control study of chronic fatigue syndrome. Exp Brain Res. 2010, 204 (1): 71-78. 10.1007/s00221-010-2296-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Keselbrener L, Akselrod S, Ahiron A, Eldar M, Barak Y, Rotstein Z: Is fatigue in patients with multiple sclerosis related to autonomic dysfunction?. Clin Auton Res. 2000, 10 (4): 169-175. 10.1007/BF02291352.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Merkelbach S, Dillmann U, Kölmel C, Holz I, Muller M: Cardiovascular autonomic dysregulation and fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler. 2001, 7 (5): 320-326.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Flachenecker P, Rufer A, Bihler I, Hippel C, Reiners K, Toyka KV, Kesselring J: Fatigue in MS is related to sympathetic vasomotor dysfunction. Neurology. 2003, 61 (6): 851-853.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Newton JL, Davidson A, Kerr S, Bhala N, Pairman J, Burt J, Jones DE: Autonomic dysfunction in primary biliary cirrhosis correlates with fatigue severity. Eur J Gastroenterol Hepatol. 2007, 19 (2): 125-132. 10.1097/01.meg.0000252629.96043.67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Chaudhuri A, Behan PO: Fatigue and basal ganglia. J Neurol Sci. 2000, 179 (1-2): 34-42. 10.1016/S0022-510X(00)00411-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Tanaka M, Mizuno K, Tajima S, Sasabe T, Watanabe Y: Central nervous system fatigue alters autonomic nerve activity. Life Sci. 2009, 84 (7-8): 235-239. 10.1016/j.lfs.2008.12.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Mezzacappa E, Kindlon D, Saul JP, Earls F: Executive and motivational control of performance task behavior, and autonomic heart-rate regulation in children: physiologic validation of two-factor solution inhibitory control. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1998, 39 (4): 525-531. 10.1017/S0021963098002273.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Johnson SA, Yechiam E, Murphy RR, Queller S, Stout JC: Motivational processes and autonomic responsivity in Asperger's disorder: evidence from the Iowa Gambling Task. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2006, 12 (5): 668-676.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Fukuda K, Straus SE, Hickie I, Sharpe MC, Dobbins JG, Komaroff A: The chronic fatigue syndrome: a comprehensive approach to its definition and study. International Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Study Group. Ann Intern Med. 1994, 121 (12): 953-959.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Ataka S, Tanaka M, Nozaki S, Mizuma H, Mizuno K, Tahara T, Sugino T, Shirai T, Kajimoto Y, Kuratsune H, Kajimoto O, Watanabe Y: Effects of applephenon (R) and ascorbic acid on physical fatigue. Nutrition. 2007, 23 (5): 419-423. 10.1016/j.nut.2007.03.002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Ataka S, Tanaka M, Nozaki S, Mizuma H, Mizuno K, Tahara T, Sugino T, Shirai T, Kajimoto Y, Kuratsune H, Kajimoto O, Watanabe Y: Effects of oral administration of caffeine and D-ribose on mental fatigue. Nutrition. 2008, 24 (3): 233-238. 10.1016/j.nut.2007.12.002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Mizuno K, Tanaka M, Nozaki S, Yamaguti K, Mizuma H, Sasabe T, Sugino T, Shirai T, Kataoka Y, Kajimoto Y, Kuratsune H, Kajimoto O, Watanabe Y: Mental fatigue-induced decrease in levels of several plasma amino acids. J Neural Transm. 2007, 114 (5): 555-561. 10.1007/s00702-006-0608-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Mizuno K, Tanaka M, Nozaki S, Mizuma H, Ataka S, Tahara T, Sugino T, Shirai T, Kajimoto Y, Kuratsune H, Kajimoto O, Watanabe Y: Antifatigue effects of coenzyme Q10 during physical fatigue. Nutrition. 2008, 24 (4): 293-299. 10.1016/j.nut.2007.12.007.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Mizuma H, Tanaka M, Nozaki S, Mizuno K, Tahara T, Ataka S, Sugino T, Shirai T, Kajimoto Y, Kuratsune H, Kajimoto O, Watanabe Y: Daily oral administration of crocetin attenuates physical fatigue in human. Nutr Res. 2009, 29 (3): 145-150. 10.1016/j.nutres.2009.02.003.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Nozaki S, Tanaka M, Mizuno K, Ataka S, Mizuma H, Tahara T, Sugino T, Shirai T, Eguchi A, Okuyama K, Yoshida K, Kajimoto Y, Kuratsune H, Kajimoto O, Watanabe Y: Mental and physical fatigue-related biochemical alterations. Nutrition. 2009, 25 (1): 51-57. 10.1016/j.nut.2008.07.010.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Kajimoto O: Development of a method of evaluation of fatigue and its economic impacts. Fatigue Science for Human Health. Edited by: Watanabe Y, Evengård B, Natelson BH, Jason LA, Kuratsune H. 2008, New York: Springer, 33-46.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Mizuno K, Watanabe Y: Utility of an advanced trail making test as a neuropsychological tool for an objective evaluation of work efficiency during mental fatigue. Fatigue Science for Human Health. Edited by: Watanabe Y, Evengård B, Natelson BH, Jason LA, Kuratsune H. 2008, New York: Springer, 47-54.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Yamamoto K: Analysis of cognitive function and regional cerebral blood flow in Parkinson's disease by 123I-IMP SPECT. Rinsho Shinkeigaku. 1992, 32 (1): 1-7.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Yoshiuchi K, Nomura S, Ando K, Ohtake T, Shimosawa T, Kumano H, Kuboki T, Suematsu H, Fujita T: Hemodynamic and endocrine responsiveness to mental arithmetic task and mirror drawing test in patients with essential hypertension. Am J Hypertens. 1997, 10 (3): 243-249. 10.1016/S0895-7061(96)00382-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Reitan RM: The relation of the trail making test to organic brain damage. J Consult Psychol. 1955, 19 (5): 393-394.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Yamaguti K: The evaluation of fatigue by using acceleration plethysmography. Nippon Rinsho. 2007, 65 (6): 1034-1042.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Tajima K, Tanaka M, Mizuno K, Okada N, Rokushima K, Watanabe Y: Effects of bathing in micro-bubbles on recovery from moderate mental fatigue. Ergonomia IJE&HF. 2008, 30 (2): 135-145.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Mizuno K, Tanaka M, Tajima K, Okada N, Rokushima K, Watanabe Y: Effects of mild-stream bathing on recovery from mental fatigue. Med Sci Monit. 2010, 16 (1): CR8-CR14.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Takada M, Ebara T, Kamijima M: Heart rate variability assessment in Japanese workers recovered from depressive disorders resulting from job stress: measurements in the workplace. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 2010, 83 (5): 521-529. 10.1007/s00420-009-0499-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Akselrod S, Gordon D, Ubel FA, Shannon DC, Barger AC, Cohen RJ: Power spectrum analysis of heart rate fluctuation: a quantitative probe of beat-to-beat cardiovascular control. Science. 1981, 213 (4504): 220-222. 10.1126/science.6166045.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Pomeranz B, Macaulay RJ, Caudill MA, Kutz I, Adam D, Gordon D, Kilborn KM, Barger AC, Shannon DC, Cohen RJ, Benson H: Assessment of autonomic function in humans by heart rate spectral analysis. Am J Physiol. 1985, 248 (1-2): 151-153.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Malliani A, Pagani M, Lombardi F, Cerutti S: Cardiovascular neural regulation explored in the frequency domain. Circulation. 1991, 84 (2): 482-492.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Appel ML, Berger RD, Saul JP, Smith JM, Cohen RJ: Beat to beat variability in cardiovascular variables: noise or music?. J Am Coll Cardiol. 1989, 14 (5): 1139-1148. 10.1016/0735-1097(89)90408-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Pagani M, Montano N, Porta A, Malliani A, Abboud FM, Birkett C, Somers VK: Relationship between spectral components of cardiovascular variabilities and direct measures of muscle sympathetic nerve activity in humans. Circulation. 1997, 95 (6): 1441-1448.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Blitz PS, Hoogstraten J, Mulder G: Mental load, heart rate and heart rate variability. Psychologische Forschung. 1970, 33 (4): 277-288. 10.1007/BF00424555.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Hyndman BW, Gregory JR: Spectral analysis of sinus arrhythmia during mental loading. Ergonomics. 1975, 18 (3): 255-270. 10.1080/00140137508931460.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Pagani M, Lombardi F, Guzzetti S, Rimoldi O, Furlan R, Pizzinelli P, Sandrone G, Malfatto G, Dell'Orto S, Piccaluga E, Turiel M, Baselli G, Cerutti S, Malliani A: Power spectral analysis of heart rate and arterial pressure variabilities as a marker of sympatho-vagal interaction in man and conscious dog. Circ Res. 1986, 59 (2): 178-193.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Pagani M, Furlan R, Pizzinelli P, Crivellaro W, Cerutti S, Malliani A: Spectral analysis of R-R and arterial pressure variabilities to assess sympatho-vagal interaction during mental stress in humans. J Hypertens Suppl. 1989, 7 (6): S14-S15.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Lucini D, Covacci G, Milani R, Mela GS, Malliani A, Pagani M: A controlled study of the effects of mental relaxation on autonomic excitatory responses in healthy subjects. Psychosom Med. 1997, 59 (5): 541-552.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Toichi M, Kamio Y: Paradoxical autonomic response to mental tasks in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2003, 33 (4): 417-426. 10.1023/A:1025062812374.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Thayer JF, Sternberg E: Beyond heart rate variability: vagal regulation of allostatic systems. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006, 1088: 361-372. 10.1196/annals.1366.014.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Thayer JF, Yamamoto SS, Brosschot JF: The relationship of autonomic imbalance, heart rate variability and cardiovascular disease risk factors. Int J Cardiol. 2010, 141 (2): 122-131. 10.1016/j.ijcard.2009.09.543.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Amat J, Baratta MV, Paul E, Bland ST, Watkins LR, Maier SF: Medial prefrontal cortex determines how stressor controllability affects behavior and dorsal raphe nucleus. Nat Neurosci. 2005, 8 (3): 365-371. 10.1038/nn1399.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Thayer JF: On the importance of inhibition: central and peripheral manifestations of nonlinear inhibitory processes in neural systems. Dose Response. 2006, 4 (1): 2-21. 10.2203/dose-response.004.01.002.Thayer.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Boksem MA, Meijman TF, Lorist MM: Mental fatigue, motivation and action monitoring. Biol Psychol. 2006, 72 (2): 123-132. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Tanaka M, Sadato N, Okada T, Mizuno K, Sasabe T, Tanabe HC, Saito DN, Onoe H, Kuratsune H, Watanabe Y: Reduced responsiveness is an essential feature of chronic fatigue syndrome: a fMRI study. BMC Neurol. 2006, 6: 9-10.1186/1471-2377-6-9.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Caseras X, Mataix-Cols D, Rimes KA, Giampietro V, Brammer M, Zelaya F, Chalder T, Godfrey E: The neural correlates of fatigue: an exploratory imaginal fatigue provocation study in chronic fatigue syndrome. Psychol Med. 2008, 38 (7): 941-951.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Okada T, Tanaka M, Kuratsune H, Watanabe Y, Sadato N: Mechanisms underlying fatigue: a voxel-based morphometric study of chronic fatigue syndrome. BMC Neurol. 2004, 4: 14-10.1186/1471-2377-4-14.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Circulation. 1996, 93 (5): 1043-1065.

  55. Piccirillo G, Ogawa M, Song J, Chong VJ, Joung B, Han S, Magrì D, Chen LS, Lin SF, Chen PS: Power spectral analysis of heart rate variability and autonomic nervous system activity measured directly in healthy dogs and dogs with tachycardia-induced heart failure. Heart Rhythm. 2009, 6 (4): 546-552. 10.1016/j.hrthm.2009.01.006.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Huibers MJ, Kant IJ, Knottnerus JA, Bleijenberg G, Swaen GM, Kasl SV: Development of the chronic fatigue syndrome in severely fatigued employees: predictors of outcome in the Maastricht cohort study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2004, 58 (10): 877-882. 10.1136/jech.2003.017939.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported in part by the Ministry of Health, Labour and Welfare of the Japan. We thank Dr. Christine Collins for editorial assistance with the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kei Mizuno.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

KM took part in designing and planning the experiment, data analyses and manuscript preparation. MT contributed to designing and planning the experiment, data analyses and manuscript preparation. KY contributed to designing and planning the experiment and analysing the data. OK and HK contributed to designing and planning the experiment. YW took part in planning and designing the experiment and preparing the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mizuno, K., Tanaka, M., Yamaguti, K. et al. Mental fatigue caused by prolonged cognitive load associated with sympathetic hyperactivity. Behav Brain Funct 7, 17 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-7-17

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-7-17

Keywords