Skip to main content

Apathy symptoms modulate motivational decision making on the Iowa gambling task

Abstract

Background

The present study represents an initial attempt to assess the role of apathy in motivated decision making on the Iowa Gambling Task. Clinical descriptions of patients with apathy highlight deficits in the cognitive, emotional and behavioural aspects of goal directed activity, yet standard neurocognitive tests of these measures fail to demonstrate reliable sensitivity to the disorder. Available research suggests the Iowa Gambling Task is a robust test of complex emotional socio-executive processes involved in motivational decision making, which can analogue real-world goal-directed behaviour.

Methods

We ask whether performance on the Iowa Gambling Task can distinguish brain damaged patients with apathy symptoms from 1) brain damaged patients without apathy and 2) neurologically intact controls. Overall, 22 healthy adults and 29 brain damaged patients took part in this study.

Results

Brain damaged patients with apathy were distinctively impaired on the Iowa Gambling Task compared to both non-apathetic brain damaged patients and neurologically intact healthy controls. On the other hand, standard measures for the cognitive control of behaviour failed to show this sensitivity.

Conclusions

Our results demonstrated that the Iowa Gambling Task is sensitive to the presence of apathy symptoms. We discuss these findings in terms of neurocognition deficits in apathy and the related implications for rehabilitation and clinical intervention.

Background

Apathy as a syndrome manifests as reductions in motivation, goal directed thoughts, emotions, and behaviour [1]. The disorder is of frequent occurrence following neurological change [2]. Rehabilitation outcome studies on chronic patients going beyond 2 years post brain injury show that the majority of cases experience persisting apathy symptoms with related psychosocial problems characterised by lack of motivation, attenuated emotionality, decreased social contact and leisure activity, unemployment, marital problems and family breakdowns [3]. Apathy is also associated with significant caregiver distress and early institutionalization [4]. Despite these serious clinical implications, the nosological position of apathy and its associated neurocognitive profile remain poorly understood and appreciated in clinical practice [1].

Neurological models suggest apathy follows dysfunction in frontal-subcortical brain circuits crucial for motivation-related executive processing [5, 6]. The classic case of Phineas Gage provides the earliest documented case of such dysfunction. Despite his preserved capacities on basic cognition after his brain injury, Gage had significant socio-executive deficits including personality change and apparent apathy to his symptoms [7]. Similar cases have been reported elsewhere, e.g., [8–11]. Apathy is often conceptualised as a dysexecutive syndrome [12], although studies have shown inconsistent results on the relationship between apathy and executive deficits assessed through standard executive function (EF) tests. Some studies have reported an association between apathy symptoms and poor performance on these tests [13, 14] while others have found no such an association. For instance, a review by van Reekum and associates [15] found a near middle split in the number of studies that reported a significant relationship between apathy levels and executive deficits (8 studies) and those that did not (7 studies).

Use of different tests and scales on different clinical samples across studies may partly account for the mixed results. Also, in some cases real-life dysexecutive behaviour has been seen to dissociate from deficits on standard EF tests [16–18]. Patients with significant functional problems often perform relatively well on these ‘offline’ tests [19]. Robust tasks that are sensitive to real life socio-executive processing may help us understand the nature of neurocognitive impairment in apathy. The socio-executive processing needed for successful Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) performance is thought to analogue real life motivated behaviour [20, 21], and therefore makes a potentially useful test for the deficits in goal directed behaviour that comprise apathy symptoms.

A number of reasons make the IGT a plausible test for apathy symptoms. Firstly, brain areas thought to subserve emotional-executive processes crucial for effective IGT performance such as the medial frontal regions, anterior cingulate cortex, and amygdala form part of the cortico-subcortical neural circuitry responsible for motivated goal direct behaviour [22–28]. Damage to these areas reliably causes apathy [29–35]. Secondly, apathy and impaired IGT performance have both been linked to malfunctions of the dopamine system. Blockade of dopamine has been reported to impair, and stimulation of dopamine to improve, IGT performance [36]. Enhanced IGT performance due to stimulation of dopamine is also consistent with the view that dopamine mediates exploratory behaviour and the suggested effectiveness of dopamine agonists in combating apathy symptoms [37, 38]. With this converging evidence, the IGT provides an attractive premise to build a theoretical framework for understanding neurocognitive correlates of apathy symptoms.

In this study we assessed whether the IGT is sensitive to the presence of apathy symptoms in patients with acquired brain damage. It is also possible that the presence of apathy symptoms may impair performance on any test as a result of general motivational deficits. For that reason, we also obtained independent EF measures on the Brixton Spatial Anticipation test [39].

Method

Participants

In total 51 participants took part in the study. Twenty nine brain damaged patients were recruited from brain injury clinics in the West Midlands region of England. Most neurocognitive studies on apathy have been done on homogenous patient samples, thus making it difficult to isolate the influence of the aetiological process, apathy symptoms, and executive processing deficits. To increase the power of the relationship between apathy symptoms and socio-executive deficits, we profiled brain damaged patients from a number of aetiologies (Cerebrovascular accident = 10; Head injury = 7; Anoxia = 5; Herpes Simplex Encephalitis = 5; Aneurysm = 2). For this reason, the patients’ lesions were also of varied locations. Lesion location data for each of the patients was obtained from brain scan information in the patients’ clinic files (See Table 1 for the lesion data).

Table 1 Lesion location and apathy diagnosis

Twenty-two healthy adult controls recruited through local adverts in the West Midlands city of Birmingham also provided the normative data on the IGT (See Table 2 for the participants’ demographic characteristics). All participants gave informed written consent to participate in the study. Ethics approval for the study was obtained from the Birmingham and Solihull Research Ethics Committee.

Table 2 Participants age and education characteristics

Apathy symptoms

The informant version of the Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES-I) [40] was used to assess apathy. The AES-I is an 18-item scale that assesses behavioural, emotional, and cognitive aspects of apathy. Each item, (e.g., s/he gets things done during the day) is rated on a scale of 1 (Not at all characteristic) to 4 (A lot characteristic). The scale has enjoyed widespread use and has good psychometric properties [40]. Patients’ caregivers provided the evaluations. We took apathy scores of 41+ as indicative of the presence of apathy (supplementary administration and scoring guidelines obtained from the authors). To control for the likely influence of depression on the relationships between study variables, the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; [41]) was also used to evaluate levels of depressive symptoms in the patient samples.

Executive function measures: the Brixton test

The Brixton Spatial Anticipation test [39] provided standard EF measures. This test was chosen because of its robustness and sensitivity to a variety of executive deficits, including preservative behaviour and failure to utilise feedback or follow rules [39].

Socio-executive measure: the Iowa gambling task

The IGT requires participants to choose cards from four decks labelled A, B, C, and D. Each deck is made up of 40 cards. The task is rigged such that cards from decks A and B give high rewards (£100) on each card selection, but also yield unpredictable and large losses such that continuously picking from these decks results in a net loss. Decks A and B are therefore risky decks. On the other hand, Decks C and D are safe. They give relatively lower immediate gains on each picking (£50), but the associated losses are also lower. Picking more cards from the safe decks C and D gives a net gain, and picking more cards from the risky decks A and B results in a net loss.

Because we used the non-automated card version of the IGT, some of the patients ran out of play money at various points after the 3rd Block (see below). Also, because the non-automated IGT version has fixed numbers of cards for each deck, better performers ran out of safe picks before reaching the final round of choices and had to pick from the risky decks out of lack of choice. For these reasons, we only analysed card choices up to the 60th round. While this is a potential limitation to our study, it is important to note that in neurologically intact subjects, the trend towards avoiding risky decks is often set by the 40th choice [21].

Results

Tests for sphericity and homogeneity of variance were performed on the data to determine whether it met the assumptions for the use of parametric tests. Our data met these assumptions.

Apathy evaluation

Nineteen patients (Male = 18, Female = 1; Mean age = 54, SD = 14.25) met criteria for the presence of apathy (Mean apathy scores = 52.8, SD = 7.4) and 10 patients (Male = 7; Female = 3; Mean age 56.5, SD = 14.17) did not have apathy (Mean = 33.2, SD = 5.5).

Gambling task

To investigate the participants’ gambling trends, we divided the rounds of choices into 3 blocks: Block 1 (rounds 1–20), Block 2 (rounds 21–40), and Block 3 (rounds 41–60). Neurologically intact controls picked fewer safe cards (from decks C and D) in Block 1 (Mean = 7.68, SD = 2.81); they then picked more safe cards in Block 2 (Mean = 14.73, SD = 3.8) and even more safe cards in Block 3 (Mean = 15.59, SD = 3.54). The same trend towards picking more from the safe C and D decks as the game progressed was also observed for non-apathetic patients (Block 1: Mean = 9.1, SD = 3.54; Block 2: Mean = 11.40, SD = 4.69; and Block 3, Mean = 13.10, SD = 6.0). In contrast, although on average apathetic patients picked more safe cards in block 1 compared to the other two groups (Mean = 10.21, SD = 3.46), they eventually picked fewer safe cards in block 2 (Mean = 8.95, SD = 4.43) and even fewer safe cards in block 3 (Mean = 8.05, SD = 4.97). Figure 1 shows the mount of safe picks (from decks C and D) for the 3 participant groups.

Figure 1
figure 1

The three graphs show less risky card selections for patients with apathy symptoms, patients without apathy and health controls across Blocks 1, 2 and 3.

A mixed ANOVA performed on the safe (C+D) scores for the 3 participant groups with Block (Blocks 1, 2, and 3) as a within subject factor and Participant Type (Normal, Non-apathetic, and Apathetic) as a between subjects variable showed significant main effects of Block F(2, 98) = 31.10, p < .001, r = .24. The interaction between Block and participant type was also significant F (2, 98) = 21.08, p < .001, r = .39. When depressive symptoms (BDI score) and executive function deficits (Brixton test scores) were controlled for as covariates for the two patient groups, there where non-significant effects of executive function deficits F (1, 21) = 1.02, p > .05, r = .22, and of depressive symptoms, F (1, 21) = .004, p > .05 r = .1. The effects of Block remained significant, F (1, 21) = 3.81, p < .05 r = .45.

Further one way ANOVA tests on risky gambling scores showed significant group differences (F (2, 48) = 9.20, p < .001 r = .41). Pairwise comparisons indicated that apathetic patients made significantly more risky card choices compared to both normal controls (Mean difference = 9.9; p < .001, r = .28) and patients without apathy (Mean difference = 5.44; p < .05, r = .18). IGT scores for the non apathetic patients were not significantly different from scores for healthy controls (Mean difference = 4.1; p > .05, r = .26). See Table 3 for these comparisons.

Table 3 Pairwise comparisons on gambling task performance between apathetic patients, non-apathetic patients, and healthy controls

There is little consensus on the appropriate cut-off score for the AES-I. Cut-off scores have ranged from 35 to 41 across studies (Glenn et al., 2002; Lane-Brown & Tate; 2009). For this reason we also performed Pearson correlation analyses between patients’ AES-I apathy ratings and the total number of risky cards they selected. The results showed a significant positive relationship between the level of apathy symptoms and the number of risky cards selected, r = .38, p < 0.05. Figure 2 shows the scatter plot for this relationship and illustrates the point that patients with high levels of apathy also picked relatively more risky cards.

Figure 2
figure 2

The relationship between levels of apathy and the number of risky card selections.

Discussion

Patients with apathy made significantly more risky choices on the IGT compared to neurologically intact and brain damaged non-apathetic controls, both of whom (as groups) picked less from the risky decks as the game progressed. The shift from risky to the safe decks shown by healthy participants is in line with the performance patterns reported in similar studies [42–47]. Non-apathetic brain damaged patients also shifted towards good decks as the game progressed.

It is possible that apathetic patients may perform poorly due to their general lack of motivation, and not necessarily due to a specific cognitive deficit related to the particular task. This argument is however inconsistent with our findings. For instance, putting in the Brixton test score as a covariate did not dilute the effect, suggesting the two patient groups’ performance on this test was not significantly different. We can claim with some degree of confidence that the IGT is sensitive to the presence of apathy symptoms, and distinguished apathetic from non-apathetic patients (and normal controls).

The IGT’s sensitivity to apathy symptoms makes it a potentially valuable instrument for both research and clinical practice. In a number of cases standard off-line EF measures fail to distinguish between patients with real-life socio-executive deficits like apathy and those without these deficits [48]. Standard EF tests may provide test takers with reliable task instructions and the type of feedback that is not found in more complex real life scenarios. On the other hand, the IGT has open ended behavioural choices and requires the generation of self-initiated choices under less explicit feedback. Such conditions provide a more valid analogue of socio-executive demands found in real life social environments. Our results suggest that apathy symptoms may arise from specific deficits in a patient’s capacity to produce or structure their own goal directed behaviour. This is in line with the realisation that apathetic patients rely heavily on others to structure their own activities [49]. Rehabilitation programmes in which daily tasks are structured, with enough prompts and cues may benefit patients with apathy. In this context, the IGT can be a useful addition to assessment batteries for detecting socio-executive deficits related to apathy.

Effective IGT performance is thought to depend on intact emotional processing [50]. According to this view (the somatic marker hypothesis), effective IGT performance benefits from subconscious emotional capacities that pre-bias responses away from bad behavioural choices. This bias enhances the selection and efficient execution of good behavioural choices, and is thought to underlie adaptive goal-directed activity, reasoning and decision-making in real life social contexts. It is tempting to suggest an inability to develop or fully utilise such emotional biasing signals to explain apathy symptoms. Empirical studies support this viewpoint. For instance, it has been observed that patients with apathy show deficits in matching emotional responses to social situations [51, 52]. Furthermore, there is evidence for a significant inverse association between apathy and autonomic excitation measured through heart rate reactivity [53]. Our study is however limited in the extent to which we can ascribe poor performance on the IGT to such lack of autonomic emotional inputs because we did not obtain concurrent measures of autonomic arousal.

The IGT’s sensitivity to apathy symptoms may also lie in its requirement for sensitivity to rewards and punishments. Part of the apathy symptom profile includes a dimension of indifference to rewards and punishments, lack of interest or concern, anhedonia, and social disengagement [54, 55]. Apathetic patients may have picked more risky cards due to insensitivity to punishments. Bechara and colleagues [56] have however demonstrated that sensitivity to rewards or insensitivity to punishment contingencies is not enough to explain impaired IGT performance. They either reversed rewards and punishments on the task such that good decks yielded higher immediate punishments but even greater delayed rewards, while risky decks gave off low immediate punishments and even lower future rewards, and in another condition increased the future disadvantages on risky decks. Patients with dysexecutive deficits preferred decks that had low immediate punishment to those with higher immediate punishment but more rewards in the long run. In other words large delayed rewards failed to lure the patients to the paying decks while on the other hand the patients were reluctant to choose from decks with huge initial punishment indicating that hyper sensitivity to rewards or insensitivity to punishments might not be a crucial factor on effective IGT performance.

Fellows and Farah [22] suggest that impaired goal directed behaviour can result from impaired future time perspective, which is a measure of an individual’s self defined future. Interestingly, deficits in future time perspective were found to correlate with self-reported apathy symptoms [22]. Whether this deficit can explain impaired IGT performance in this study is a subject for future research. There are also some suggestions that deficits in reversal learning or inhibition could account for impairments on the IGT. Effective performance requires that participants switch from initially high paying decks to lower paying but also low punishment decks. This involves reversal learning and the inhibition of responses to high reward/high cost decks - capacities that can be impaired in some brain damaged patients [57]. It is unlikely that deficits in reversal learning or inhibition could account for our results though, since apathetic patients were not distinctly impaired on the Brixton test.

Other factors, such as the effects of working memory deficits on IGT performance [58], and the cognitive impenetrability of the task [59, 60] have been suggested to explain impaired performance on the task. However, Bechara, et al. [61] have shown that working memory and decision making are dissociable and also shown that the majority of normal participants begin avoiding risky decks before confessing awareness of the game’s rewards and punishments contingencies. See also [62].

The IGT’s main weakness is its lack of specificity. A majority of different patient populations have shown deficits on the measure. Impaired IGT performance has been reported in schizophrenics [63–65]; in pathological gamblers [66]; in alcohol, marijuana, and substance dependent individuals [67–69]; in anorexics [70] in patients with intermittent explosive disorder and conduct disorder [71, 72]; in Huntington’s disease sufferers [24]; in traumatic brain injury patients [73]; in frontotemporal dementia [74] and in HIV infected patients [75]. It is nevertheless important to note that apathy is also a common syndrome in most of these disorders [15].

The IGT’s lack of specificity across different neuropsychiatric disorders could also be an indication of the task’s sensitivity to a wider spectrum of deficits, for instance, involving working memory, the patient’s interest and concern about the game, the processing of reward and punishment, explorative behaviour, and capacities related to the inhibition of choices on highly paying but risky decks.

Conclusions

The IGT’s sensitivity to apathy symptoms makes it a potentially valuable neurocognitive tool for assessing these symptoms. However the future utility of the task rests upon our ability to isolate the potential causes of impaired performance in specific clinical populations and syndromes. This should help unravel the individual aspects of the task that, for instance, give it sensitivity to apathy symptoms. Future studies may try and isolate or control for these various variables that can potentially impair IGT performance. Future studies may also consider using the electronic version of the IGT which allows the administration of the full 100 rounds even with poor or extremely good performers.

References

  1. Robert P, Onyike CU, Leentjens AF, Dujardin K, Aalten P, Starkstein S: Proposed diagnostic criteria for apathy in Alzheimer's disease and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Eur Psychiatry. 2009, 24: 98-104. 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2008.09.001.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Feil D, Razani J, Boone K, Lesser I: Apathy and cognitive performance in older adults with depression. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2003, 18: 479-485. 10.1002/gps.869.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Morton VM, Wehman P: Psychosocial and emotional sequelae of individuals with traumatic brain injury: a literature review and recommendations. Brain Inj. 1995, 9: 81-92. 10.3109/02699059509004574.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Tun SM, Murman DL, Long HL, Colenda CC, von Eye A: Predictive validity of neuropsychiatric subgroups on nursing home placement and survival in patients with Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007, 15: 780-789. 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31805d858a.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Chase TN: Apathy in neuropsychiatric disease: diagnosis, pathophysiology, and treatment. Neurotox Res. 2011, 19: 266-78. 10.1007/s12640-010-9196-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Tekin S, Cummings JL: Frontal–subcortical neuronal circuits and clinical neuropsychiatry: an update. J Psychosom Res. 2002, 53: 647-654. 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00428-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. MacMillan M: An Odd Kind of Fame: Stories of Phineas Gage. 2000, Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press

    Google Scholar 

  8. Damasio AR: Descartes Error: Emotion. 1994, Reason and the Human Brain, Avon, New York

    Google Scholar 

  9. Dimitrov M, Phipps M, Zahn T, Grafman J: A thoroughly modern gage. Neurocase. 1999, 5: 345-354. 10.1080/13554799908411987.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Saver JL, Damasio AR: Preserved access and processing of social knowledge in a patient with acquired sociopathy due to ventromedial frontal damage. Neuropsychologia. 1991, 1991 (29): 1241-1249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Tranel D: The Iowa-Benton School of neuropsychological assessment. Neuropsychological assessment of neuropsychiatric disorders. Edited by: Grant I, Adams KM. 1994, New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  12. Mesulum M: The human frontal lobes: Transcending the default mode through contingent encoding. Principles of frontal Function. Edited by: Stuss DR, Knight R. 2002, New York: Oxford University Press, 8-30.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  13. Drijgers RL, Verhey FR, Leentjens AF, Köhler S, Aalten P: Neuropsychological correlates of apathy in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease: the role of executive functioning. Int Psychogeriatr. 2011, 23: 1327-33. 10.1017/S1041610211001037.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Kuzis G, Sabe L, Tiberti C, Dorrego F, Starkstein SE: Neuropsychological correlates of apathy and depression in patients with dementia. Neurol. 1999, 52: 1403-1407. 10.1212/WNL.52.7.1403.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. van Reekum R, Stuss DT, Ostrander L: Apathy: why care?. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2005, 17: 7-19. 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.17.1.7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Shallice T, Burgess PW: Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain. 1991, 114: 727-41. 10.1093/brain/114.2.727.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Stuss DT, Buckle L: Traumatic brain injury: neuropsychological deficits and evaluation at different stages of recovery and in different pathologic subtypes. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 1992, 7: 40-9. 10.1097/00001199-199206000-00007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Damasio AR, Tranel D, Damasio H: Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior: theory and preliminary testing. Frontal lobe function and dysfunction. Edited by: Levin HS, Eisenberg HM, Benton AL. 1991, New York: Oxford UP, 217-229.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Lezak MD: Neuropsychological Assessment. 1995, New York: Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  20. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW: Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cogn. 1994, 50: 7-15. 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR: Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997, 275: 1293-1295. 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Fellows LK, Farah MJ: Dissociable elements of human foresight: a role for the ventromedial frontal lobes in framing the future, but not in discounting future rewards. Neuropsychologia. 2005, 43: 1214-1221. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.07.018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Tranel D, Bechara A, Damasio AR: Decision making and the somatic marker hypothesis. The New Cognitive Neurosciences. Edited by: Gazzaniga MS. 1999, Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press, 1047-1061.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Campbell MC, Stout JC, Finn PR: Reduced autonomic responsiveness to gambling task losses in Huntington’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004, 10: 239-245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Kleeberg J, Bruggimann MA, Annoni J, Melle G, Bougousslavsky MD, Schluep M: Altered decision-making in multiple sclerosis: a sign of impaired emotional reactivity. Ann Neurol. 2004, 56: 787-795. 10.1002/ana.20277.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR, Lee GP: Different contributions of the human amygdala and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex to decision-making. J Neurosci. 1999, 19: 5473-5481.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR: An anatomical system subserving decision-making [abstract]. Abstr Soc Neurosci. 1997, 23: 495-

    Google Scholar 

  28. Barrash J, Tranel D, Anderson SW: Acquired personality disturbances associated with bilateral damage to the ventromedial prefrontal region. Dev Neuropsychol. 2000, 18: 355-381. 10.1207/S1532694205Barrash.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Dunn BD, Dalgeish T, Lawrence AD: The somatic marker hypothesis: a critical evaluation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006, 30: 239-271. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.07.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Brown RG, Pluck G: Negative symptoms: the ‘pathology’ of motivation and goal-directed behaviour. Trends Neurosci. 2000, 23: 412-417. 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01626-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Cummings JL: On: frontal-subcortical circuits and human behaviour. J Psychosom Res. 1998, 44: 627-628.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Haznedar MM, Buchsbaum MS, Luu C, Hazlett EA, Siegel BV, Lohr J, Wu J, Haier RJ, Bunney WE: Decreased anterior cingulate gyrus metabolic rate in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1997, 154: 682-684.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Mega MS, Cummings JL: The cingulate and cingulate syndromes. Contemporary Behavioral Neurology. Edited by: Trimble MR, Cummings JR. 1997, Boston: Butterworth-Heinemann, 189-214.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Saint-Cyr JA, Bronstein YL, Cummings JL: Neurobehavioural consequences of neurosurgical treatments and focal lesions of frontal-subcortical circuits. Principles of frontal Function. Edited by: Stuss D, Knight R. 2002, New York: Oxford University Press, 428-447.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Spence SA, Frith CD: Towards a functional anatomy of volition. J Consciousness Stud. 1999, 6: 11-29.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio AR: Manipulation of dopamine and serotonin causes different effects on covert and overt decision-making [abstract]. Abstr Soc Neurosci. 2001, 27: 126-

    Google Scholar 

  37. Powell JH, Al-Adawi S, Morgan J, Greenwood RJ: Motivational deficits after brain injury: effects of bromocriptine in 11 patients. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1996, 60: 416-421. 10.1136/jnnp.60.4.416.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Watanabe MD, Martin EM, DeLeon OA, Gaviria M, Pavel DG, Trepashko DW: Successful methyphenidate treatment of apathy after subcortical infarcts. J Neuropsychiatr. 1995, 7: 502-504.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Burgess PW, Shallice T: The Hayling and Brixton Tests. 1997, Thurston, Suffolk: Thames Valley Test Company

    Google Scholar 

  40. Marin RS, Biedrzycki RC, Firinciogullari S: Reliability and validity of the apathy evaluation scale. Psychiatry Res. 1991, 38: 143-162. 10.1016/0165-1781(91)90040-V.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Beck AT: Beck Depression Inventory. 1978, San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation

    Google Scholar 

  42. Apkarian AV, Sosa Y, Krauss BR, Thomas PS, Fredrickson BE, Levy RE, Harden R, Chialvo DR: Chronic pain patients are impaired on an emotional decision-making task. Pain. 2004, 108: 129-136. 10.1016/j.pain.2003.12.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Bechara A, Damasio AR: The somatic marker hypothesis: a neural theory of economic decision. Games Econ Behav. 2005, 52: 336-372. 10.1016/j.geb.2004.06.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR: Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex. 1996, 6: 215-225. 10.1093/cercor/6.2.215.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Clark L, Iversen SD, Goodwin GM: A neuropsychological investigation of prefrontal cortex involvement in acute mania. Am J Psychiatry. 2001, 158: 1605-1611. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.10.1605.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Ernst M, Kimes AS, London ED, Matochik JA, Eldreth D, Tata S: Neural substrates of decision making in adults with attentional deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2003, 160: 1061-1070. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1061.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Jollant F, Bellivier F, Leboyer M, Astruc B, Torres S, Verdier R: Impaired decision making in suicide attempters. A J Psychiatry. 2005, 162: 304-310. 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.2.304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Hodges JR: Overview of frontotemporal dementia. The Frontotemporal Dementia Syndromes. Edited by: Hodges JR. 2007, Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 1-23.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Marin RS: Apathy and related disorders of diminished motivation. American Psychiatry Association Review of Psychiatry. Edited by: Dickstein LJ, Riba MB, Oldham JM. 1996, Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, Inc, 205--242.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Damasio AR: The Feeling of What Happens: Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. 1999, New York: Harcourt

    Google Scholar 

  51. Pluck GC, Brown RG: Apathy in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002, 73: 636-642. 10.1136/jnnp.73.6.636.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Rabkin JG, Ferrando SJ, Gorp W, Rieppi R, McElhiney M, Sewell M: Relationships among apathy, depression, and cognitive impairment in HIV/AIDS. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2000, 12: 451-457. 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.12.4.451.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Andersson S, Krogstad J, Finset A: Apathy and depressed mood in acquired brain damage: relationship to lesion localization and psychophysiological reactivity. Psychol Med. 1999, 29: 447-456. 10.1017/S0033291798008046.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Landes AM, Sperry SD, Strauss ME, Geldmacher DS: Apathy in Alzheimer's disease. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2001, 49: 1700-1707. 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49282.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Marin RS: Apathy- Who cares? an introduction to apathy and related disorders of diminished motivation. Psychiatr Ann. 1997, 27: 18-23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H: Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain. 2000, 123: 2189-2202. 10.1093/brain/123.11.2189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Rolls ET, Hornak J, Wade D, McGrath J: Emotion-related learning in patients with social and emotional changes associated with frontal lobe damage. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994, 57: 1518-1524. 10.1136/jnnp.57.12.1518.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Jameson TL, Hinson JM, Whitney P: Components of working memory and somatic markers in decision making. Psychon Bull Rev. 2004, 11: 515-520. 10.3758/BF03196604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Bowman CH, Evans CEY, Turnbull OH: Artificial time-constraints on the Iowa gambling task: the effects on behavioral performance and subjective experience. Brain Cogn. 2005, 57: 21-25. 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Maia TV, McClelland JL: A re-examination of the evidence for the somatic marker hypothesis: what participants really know in the Iowa gambling task. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2004, 45: 16075-16080.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  61. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Anderson SW: Dissociation of working memory from decision-making within the human prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 1998, 18: 428-437.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Bechara A, Martin EM: Impaired decision making related to working memory deficits in individuals with substance addictions. Neuropsychology. 2004, 18: 152-162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Ritter LM, Meador-Woodruff JH, Dalack GW: Neurocognitive measures of prefrontal cortical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2004, 68: 65-73. 10.1016/S0920-9964(03)00086-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Shurman B, Horan WP, Nuechterlein KH: Schizophrenia patients demonstrate a distinctive pattern of decision-making impairment on the Iowa Gambling Task. Schizophr Res. 2005, 72: 215-224. 10.1016/j.schres.2004.03.020.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Nakamura M, Nestor PG, Levitt JJ, Cohen AS, Kawashima T, Shenton ME: Orbitofrontal volume deficit in schizophrenia and thought disorder. Brain. 2008, 131: 180-195.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Goudriaan AE, Oosterlaan J, de Beurs E, van den Brink W: Decision making in pathological gambling: a comparison between pathological gamblers, alcohol dependents, persons with Tourette syndrome, and normal controls. Cogn Brain Res. 2005, 23: 137-151. 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Bechara A, Damasio H: Decision-making and addiction (part I): impaired activation of somatic states in substance dependent individuals when pondering decisions with negative future consequences. Neuropsychologia. 2002, 40: 1675-1689. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00015-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Whitlow CT, Liguori A, Livengood LB, Hart SL, Mussat-Whitlow BJ, Lamborn CM: Long-term heavy marijuana users make costly decisions on a gambling task. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2004, 76: 107-111. 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.04.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Barry D, Petry NM: Predictors of decision-making on the Iowa gambling task: independent effects of lifetime history of substance use disorders and performance on the trail making test. Brain Cogn. 2004, 66: 243-252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Cavedini P, Bassi T, Zorzi C, Bellodi L: The advantages of choosing antiobsessive therapy according to decision-making functioning. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2004, 24: 628-631. 10.1097/01.jcp.0000144889.51072.03.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Best M, Williams JM, Coccaro EF: Evidence for a dysfunctional prefrontal circuit in patients with an impulsive aggressive disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2002, 12: 8448-8453.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  72. Fairchild G: Decision making and executive function in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder and control subjects. Brain. 2009, 62: 162-168.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Levine B, Black SE, Cheung G, Campbell A, O’Toole C, Schwartz ML: Gambling task performance in traumatic brain injury. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2005, 18: 45-54. 10.1097/01.wnn.0000152227.13001.c3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Torralva T, Roca M, Gleichgerrcht E, Bekinschtein T, Manes F: A neuropsychological battery to detect specific executive and social cognitive impairments in early frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2009, 132: 1299-309. 10.1093/brain/awp041.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Martin EM, Pitrak DL, Weddington W, Rains NA, Nunnally G, Nixon H: Cognitive impulsivity and HIV serostatus in substance dependent males. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004, 10: 931-938.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The research was supported by grants from the University of Birmingham, the University of Cape Town, the Birmingham and Solihull Mental Health Foundation NHS Trust, the Medical Research Council, the Stroke Association, and The Canon Collins Educational Trust for Southern Africa. We thank all the participants who took part in this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Progress Njomboro.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

All authors made significant contributions to the study. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Njomboro, P., Deb, S. & Humphreys, G.W. Apathy symptoms modulate motivational decision making on the Iowa gambling task. Behav Brain Funct 8, 63 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-8-63

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-8-63

Keywords